Animal personalities can trip up science, but there's a solution
">
Several years ago, Christian Rutz started to wonder whether he was giving his crows enough credit. Rutz, a biologist at the University of St. Andrews in Scotland, and his team were capturing wild New Caledonian crows and challenging them with puzzles made from natural materials before releasing them again. In one test, birds faced a log drilled with holes that contained hidden food, and could get the food out by bending a plant stem into a hook. If a bird didn't try within 90 minutes, the researchers removed it from the dataset.
But, Rutz says, he soon began to realize he was not, in fact, studying the skills of New Caledonian crows. He was studying the skills of only a subset of New Caledonian crows that quickly approached a weird log they'd never seen before--maybe because they were especially brave, or reckless.
The team changed its protocol. They began giving the more hesitant birds an extra day or two to get used to their surroundings, then trying the puzzle again. "It turns out that many of these retested birds suddenly start engaging," Rutz says. "They just needed a little bit of extra time."
Scientists are increasingly realizing that animals, like people, are individuals. They have distinct tendencies, habits and life experiences that may affect how they perform in an experiment. That means, some researchers argue, that much published research on animal behavior may be biased. Studies claiming to show something about a species as a whole--that green sea turtles migrate a certain distance, say, or how chaffinches respond to the song of a rival--may say more about individual animals that were captured or housed in a certain way, or that share certain genetic features. That's a problem for researchers who seek to understand how animals sense their environments, gain new knowledge and live their lives.
"The samples we draw are quite often severely biased," Rutz says. "This is something that has been in the air in the community for quite a long time."
In 2020, Rutz and his colleague Michael Webster, also at the University of St. Andrews, proposed a way to address this problem. They called it STRANGE.
Personalities aren't just for people
Why "STRANGE"? In 2010, an article in Behavioral and Brain Sciences suggested that the people studied in much of published psychology literature are WEIRD--drawn from Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic societies--and are "among the least representative populations one could find for generalizing about humans." Researchers might draw sweeping conclusions about the human mind when really they've studied only the minds of, say, undergraduates at the University of Minnesota.
A decade later, Rutz and Webster, drawing inspiration from WEIRD, published a paper in the journal Nature called " How STRANGE are your study animals?"
They proposed that their fellow behavior researchers consider several factors about their study animals, which they termed Social background, Trappability and self-selection, Rearing history, Acclimation and habituation, Natural changes in responsiveness, Genetic makeup, and Experience.
"I first began thinking about these kinds of biases when we were using mesh minnow traps to collect fish for experiments," Webster says. He suspected--and then confirmed in the lab-- that more active sticklebacks were more likely to swim into these traps. "We now try to use nets instead," Webster says, to catch a wider variety of fish.
That's Trappability. Other factors that might make an animal more trappable than its peers, besides its activity level, include a bold temperament, a lack of experience or simply being hungrier for bait.
Other research has shown that pheasants housed in groups of five performed better on a learning task (figuring out which hole contained food) than those housed in groups of only three--that's Social background. Jumping spiders raised in captivity were less interested in prey than wild spiders (Rearing history), and honeybees learned best in the morning (Natural changes in responsiveness). And so on.